WHAT AND WHERE IS HEAVEN?

Does heaven exist? With well over 100,000 plus recorded and described spiritual experiences collected over 15 years, to base the answer on, science can now categorically say yes. Furthermore, you can see the evidence for free on the website allaboutheaven.org.

Available on Amazon
https://www.amazon.com/dp/B086J9VKZD
also on all local Amazon sites, just change .com for the local version (.co.uk, .jp, .nl, .de, .fr etc.)

VISIONS AND HALLUCINATIONS

This book, which covers Visions and hallucinations, explains what causes them and summarises how many hallucinations have been caused by each event or activity. It also provides specific help with questions people have asked us, such as ‘Is my medication giving me hallucinations?’.

Available on Amazon
https://www.amazon.com/dp/B088GP64MW 
also on all local Amazon sites, just change .com for the local version (.co.uk, .jp, .nl, .de, .fr etc.)


Observations placeholder

North, Stephen - Spoons do not usually bend uniformly along their length; they bend at one particular place where the centre of action is concentrated

Identifier

026949

Type of Spiritual Experience

Background

A description of the experience

THE METAL-BENDERS by PROFESSOR JOHN B. HASTED

I determined to conduct further experiments with Stephen North designed to throw light on the dependence of spontaneous dynamic strain signal magnitudes on distance from the subject. I found that with Stephen it is much slower to experiment with each strain gauge mounted on its individual metal strip than with an array of strain gauges mounted on a single strip. Synchronous data are much less frequent with individual strips.

If the metal strip is chosen to be sufficiently thin, as has been shown by experiments in chapter 6, the dynamic strains can be regarded as mainly bending rather than stretching. Although they are dynamic they do not propagate extensively as shock waves along the strip. This can readily be demonstrated for normal pulses, produced artificially at the position of one of an array of strain gauges; the other strain gauges do not show significant pulses. Such an array can also be used for finding how the paranormal pulses are distributed along the metal strip. Are they all centred on the point at which the strip subsequently bends (if it does)? Or are they uniformly distributed along its length? Are the stronger ones at the end closest to the subject, or at the other end? Are they random?

In one of my first sessions with Stephen North a long metal strip carrying three strain gauges was mounted in the radial horizontal configuration, and data were recorded. After an unsettled period, the signals settled down into synchronous extension pulses, there being more than fifty such events, many of them multiple. It appeared in the first experiment that the strengths of the signals diminished with increasing distance from Stephen.

During the signals Stephen was sitting with his left hand about four inches from the end of the metal strip; he was relaxed, but refraining from movement as far as possible. I ensured that the three strain gauges were all approximately equally sensitive; this was checked by placing the entire metal strip under tension and recording the three signals. Corrections were applied if this was not the case, but in practice they were seldom more than ± 5 per cent. (Similar corrections were applied in the back-to-back strain gauge experiments described in chapter 6.)

In subsequent sessions with Stephen I repeated this distance effect experiment. But the results were rather different from those obtained in the first experiment.

In general the centre strain gauge responded most strongly, while the strain gauge nearest to Stephen received rather weaker signals. I performed further experiments with three strain gauges, and later with five and six mounted on a single specimen; for each event the relative magnitudes of the signals at each strain gauge were different. It became clear to me that any ‘distance effect’ which might exist could best be described in terms of a ‘region of action’ and a ‘centre of action’.

At this centre the signals are strongest, and they diminish on each side of it. Thus in the first session the centre was between the subject and the nearest strain gauge, and in later sessions it was closer to the centre strain gauge. Such a concept would be consistent with qualitative observations on metal-bending; spoons do not usually bend uniformly along their length; they bend at one particular place where the centre of action is concentrated. I have on occasion observed metal strips bend into exact parabolic arcs, once as long as 40 cm, indicating a bending moment uniform along the entire length; but this type of behaviour is comparatively rare (see chapter 9).

How does the peak signal strength I vary with distances from the centre of action? A detailed analysis requires more than three strain gauges, but at first I was satisfied with fitting the data to a standard probability relationship. l used a simple Gaussian equation:
I = l0*exp {-alpha*(x – x0 )^2 }
This equation represents a bell-shaped curve of width inversely related to the parameter alpha. and centred on a point x = x0, which is the actual ‘centre of action’. The variable x represents the distance from the end of the metal strip which is closest to the subject.

Each triplet of signals (or later quintet or sextet of signals) is computer fitted to this equation with different values of I0, x0 and alpha. If the variation of signal strength over the entire metal strip were to follow this behaviour (which is the simplest assumption), the implication is that the strain signals occur in a region of variable size, centred on a point of variable position; and they are of variable strength. These variations lie within certain limits described by normal statistics; probable errors e have been calculated; and a histogram of x0 values during a session is shown in Figure 8.1.

I repeated the three strain gauge experiment some eight times, with Stephen North and also with Mark Henry and Julie Knowles. The metal strips were of different lengths, and were arranged in three different orientations: radially from the subject, in a horizontal plane, RH, with the end equidistant from the subject in a horizontal plane, EH; and vertically, V. In each orientation the behaviour was similar; there is repeatability.

The results of fittings of different sessions with Stephen North are analysed in Table 8.1. The simple conception of a centre of action, with Gaussian variation of signal strengths, is vindicated. The extent of the region of action is, as we have seen, determined by the inverse of the parameter alpha. And we do not yet know how alpha depends upon length of specimen, since this has not as yet been varied much.

Of course I do not suggest that an exact Gaussian profile is maintained over the length of the metal strip. A single Gaussian equation with positive alpha can always be fitted to any three signals (provided they are convex; concave configuration yields negative alpha) and there is no question of over-determination. The data from five and six in-line strain gauges show the extent of the scatter; they fit much less precisely on a single Gaussian curve, as can be seen from the two examples represented in Figure 8.2. These are chosen from among the best and worst fits to signal quintets in session SN DD; the quality of the fit is related to an error parameter e = {((I – I0)^2)/n}^(1/2)/I0, and this is tabulated together with the Gaussian parameters in Table 8.2. A good idea of the grouping of signal magnitudes within a single session (Stephen North DD) can be obtained from this Table.

The concept of a region of action has been investigated in detail only within a single metal specimen, since, as we have seen, Stephen North and other metal-benders obtain fewer synchronized signals on several individual metal specimens. This reduction in numbers was shown very clearly in an experiment with Stephen in which I mounted three strain gauges on a single piece of metal containing two thin sections. I recorded signals, and then cut through the thin sections without seriously changing the relative positions of the remaining metal strips; there were now three pieces of metal, each with its own strain gauge; the frequency of occurrence of signals was reduced by a factor of two, and the frequency of occurrence of synchronous signals by a factor of twenty; nevertheless the strength of the signals was not seriously reduced, and one of the metal strips became permanently deformed; it appears that Stephen’s surface of action is greatly stabiised by the surface of the metal; at the termination of the metal strip, the surface becomes less accurately aligned.

Figure 8.1 Histogram of distribution of values of the action xO along aluminium strip in Stephen North’s session G.

Table 8.1 Analysis of distance effect data

Session

I

No. of signal triplets accepted

No. of signal triplets rejected

mean I0

eI0/I0

mean x0

ex0/mean x0

mean alpha

e-alpha/mean alpha

Length (cm)

Width (mm)

Thickness (mm)

Configuration

No. of visible deformations

C

0.10

131

16

17.06

0.442

24.8

0.306

0.0041

0.244

40

7.5

0.75

EH

6

L

0.29

59

15

9.07

0.655

8.31

0.194

0.054

0.443

15

7.5

0.75

RH

6

B

0.75

91

22

15.71

0.352

6.03

1.71

0.0018

0.453

40

7.5

0.75

RH

3

G

0.11

76

3

13.68

0.397

10.15

0.348

0.0055

0.220

40

7.5

0.75

RH

1

K

0.12

18

48

3.28

0.692

14.95

0.097

0.0203

0.211

30

7.5

0.75

V

1

In distance effect experiments the same feature is present as was noticed in all the earlier data: the constant changing sign of the signals. All strain gauges were mounted on the same face of the metal strip, but we observed a medley of extension signals interspersed with contraction signals. The moments of change from extension to contraction appeared to be irregular, and we have not been able to associate them with any particular psychological moment during the session. Suppose that the changing represents indecision: first the ‘attempt’ is made to bend the metal one way, then, when no permanent deformation is seen, an ‘attempt’ is made to bend it the other way. We characterize the session by its ‘indecision factor’, I, defined in chapter 6 as the number of changes expressed as a fraction of the total number of signals or ‘events’. Values of I for the distance effect experiments appear in Table 8.1. Apart from the high value of 0.75 in session SN B. a value around 10-20 per cent is common.

Figure 8.2 Gaussian curve-fitting to two signal quintets from five strain gauges mounted along a single aluminium strip, whose position and length correspond with the x axis. The quality of fit is seen to be related to the error parameter e. Session DD, Stephen North.

Table 8.2

Signal no.

I0 (x O.2mV)

alpha

xO (cm)

e

1

35.9

0.029

11.4

0.15

2

10.7

0.012

11.9

0.14

3

31.5

0.035

10.1

0.074

4

72.9

0.066

7.9

0.056

5

23.5

0.102

11.9

0.050

6

46.0

0.045

7.7

0.16

7

8.4

0.026

9.3

0.11

8

4.4

0.025

8.0

0.16

9

44.0

0.055

10.5

0.16

10

43.3

0.044

7.2

0.088

11

7.0

0.017

6.5

0.13

12

6.5

0.024

10.6

0.075

13

37.7

0.027

8.1

0.082

Since the metal-bender is not required to hold his hands absolutely still during a session, it is difficult to assess with accuracy the distance of the centre of action from the body. But I can make the following generalization. It is the perception of the target by the metal-bender which is the important factor; once one has perceived it and succeeded in producing signals, the centre of action apparently moves independently of the motion of the hands or body. It assists most metal-benders to hold their hands near the target in order to obtain the first signals; some but not all prefer to keep their hand close to the metal during the entire session. The attitude of the metal-bender depends on many factors, such as previous practice, psychological mood, the witnessing, the environment and the condition of the target. Distances sufficiently great to inhibit the perception appear to hinder the production of signals. The walls of the room are usually an important factor; strain gauges outside the room in which the subject is positioned very seldom receive signals. Experiments in the open air might reveal surprisingly different ‘perceptual distance effects’.
 

The source of the experience

North, Stephen

Concepts, symbols and science items

Concepts

Symbols

Activities and commonsteps

Activities

Suppressions

Being a child
Biofeedback

Commonsteps

Spoon bending

References